Difference between revisions of "Purépecha/grammar"

From LING073
Jump to: navigation, search
Line 84: Line 84:
 
|-
 
|-
 
|Interrogative
 
|Interrogative
|-Ø or -ki
+
|Ø or ki suffix
 
|-
 
|-
 
|Subjunctive
 
|Subjunctive
|-ka
+
|ka suffix
 
|}
 
|}
  

Revision as of 22:26, 7 March 2021

Purépechan Grammar

Parts of Speech

Nouns

Singular

These forms take no further nominal morphology apart from the appropriate case markers, such as the objective -ni or the plural marker -cha.

wari<n><sg> (woman) kurú<n><sg> (fish) anátapu<n><sg> (tree)

Plural

Irrespective of their formation method, nouns are pluralised with the suffix -echa, -icha, or -cha (depending on the variety).

wari<n><pl> ↔ wariecha (women) kurú<n><pl> ↔ kurūcha (fishes) anátapu<n><pl> ↔ anátapuicha (trees)

Case

In Purépecha, the nominative case (unmarked) is a coding property of subjects, while the suffix -ni is the case marker for objects, both in monotransitive and ditransitive constructions.

ganádu<n><pl><obj> ↔ ganáduni (cattle) kurú<n><pl><obj> ↔ kurúchani (fish) yurhíri<n><sg><obj> ↔ yurhírini (blood)

Verbs

Purépechan is a very agglutinative language, meaning that the words in the language are made by stringing together morphemes. A large portion of the agglutination happens in the verbs. Adding suffixes to the words can help users express locative, directional, causative, voice/valency, desiderative, adverbial, third person plural object, aspect, tense, irrealis, mood, and person and number. A few examples we:

kw’i<v><pres><sub><p1><sg> ↔ kw’ixakani

kw’i<v><pres><ass><p1><sg> ↔ kw’ixakani

eshé<v><pprs><pres><1/2ind><p1><sg> ↔ eshéshakani

jwá<v><perf><past><3ind> ↔ jwáspti

Roots and Agents

The suffix-nominalising suffix, most frequently -kwa (-ka in some varieties) as in pire-kwa; -ri, the most common nominalising suffix, generally refers to an agent, as in pire-ri 'singer'.

pire<inf> (to sing) pire<n><sg> ↔ pirekwa (song) pire<n><sg> ↔ pireri (singer)

Aspect

Aspect and tense markers are the same for all persons, but the indicative mood morpheme changes from -ka for the 1st and 2nd person to -ti for the 3rd person. Aspect co-occurs with tense and mood.

yontki wanta-na-xïn-an-ti juchari anapu before speak-PASS-HAB-PST-3.ASS 1PL.POSS language 'Before, our language was spoken.'

ximpóka xuchá/María t’iré-s-p-ka ya because 1pl/Maria eat-prf-pst-sbjv already ‘ . . . because we have already eaten/Maria has already eaten.’

éka t’ú/tumpí-icha xanó-∅-p-ka ya when 2sg/boy-pl arrive-prf-pst-sbjv already ‘ . . . when you had already arrived/the boys had already arrived.’

Irrealis

ni-a-ti go-IRR-3SG (He will go) ni<v><irr><p3><sg> ↔ niati

ewa-a-a-ka remove-3PL.O-IRR-1/2.ASS ('I will remove them.') ewa<v><3PL.O><irr><ass><p1><sg> ↔ ewaaaka

Mood

Purépecha the verbal inflection only distinguishes the person features of the subject in the indicative mood. The indicative verbal forms must be inflected in agreement with the grammatical subject: if the subject is 1st or 2nd person, the verb is marked with -ka; if it is 3rd person, then the marker is -ti. In contrast with the interrogative/clarificational inflection, marked with -∅ or -ki for all persons, and the subjunctive mood, marked with -ka regardless of the person of the subject.

Mood
Indicative nothing
Interrogative Ø or ki suffix
Subjunctive ka suffix

Atá-s-Ø-ka strike-prf-prs-1/2ind Atá<v><prf><pres><ind><p1> ↔ Atáska Atá<v><prf><pres><ind><p2> ↔ Atáska Atá<v><prf><pres><ind><p3> ↔ Atásti

xupá-s-Ø-ka=ni wash-prf-prs-1/2ind=1sg.sbj xupá<v><prf><pres><sg><sbj><p1> ↔ xupáskani

xanó-s-Ø-ka=ni arrive-prf-prs-1/2ind=1sg.sbj xanó<v><prf><pres><sg><sbj><p1> ↔ xanóskani


The object suffix -a is considered a 3rd person plural marker, which, according to some authors, has a pronominal or agreement value.

Person and Number

Aspect and tense markers are the same for all persons, but the indicative mood morpheme changes from -ka for the 1st and 2nd person to -ti for the 3rd person.

eró+ka-sha-ka wait+FOR-PROGR-1/2IND ‘I / you / we / you(pl) are waiting’

eró+ka-sha-ti wait+FOR-PROGR-3IND ‘He/they are waiting’

If one wishes to be more specific about the subject number, a set of clitics can be optionally attached to the inflected verb:

eró+ka-sha-ka=ni wait+FOR-PROGR-1/2IND=1SG ‘I am waiting’

eró+ka-sha-ka=ri wait+FOR-PROGR-1/2IND=2SG ‘You are waiting’ eró+ka-sha-ti wait+FOR-PROGR-3IND ‘He/they are waiting’

eró+ka-sha-ka=tsï wait+FOR-PROGR-1/2IND=2PL ‘You (pl) are waiting’

eró+ka-sha-ka=ksï wait+FOR-PROGR-1/2IND=1PL ‘We are waiting’

eró+ka-sha-ti=ksï wait+FOR-PROGR-3IND=3PL ‘They are waiting’

Others

Determinants or Determiners

Purépecha does not have a definite determiner like English "the" or Spanish "el/los." Among the most common demonstratives are the singular forms í 'this', inté 'that (visible to the speaker)', and imá 'that (not visible to the speaker)', as well as the corresponding plural forms ts'ï 'these', ts'ïmí 'those', and ts'ïmá 'those'.

Determiners
iámindu 'all'
máru 'some'
imá 'that' (not visible to the speaker)
í 'this'
inté 'that' (visible to the speaker)
ts'ï 'these'
ts'ïmí 'those'
ts'ïmá 'those'

Examples: í atá-s-∅-ti inté-ni this strike<prf><prs><p3><ind> that<obj> 'This (one) struck that (one).'

ts'ïmá atá-s-∅-ti inté-ni those strike<prf><prs><p3><ind> that<obj> 'Those struck that (one).'

ihtsï iámindu juát-icha júkska-kata jarha-h-ti DEM.PL all hill-PL sow-PPLE be-PFVE-3IND ‘All these hills are sown (with corn)’

*ihtsï maru juát-icha júkska-kata jarha-h-ti DEM.PL some hill-PL sow-PPLE be-PFVE-3IND

ihtsï tanímu juát-icha júkska-kata jarha-h-ti DEM.PL three hill-PL sow-PPLE be-PFVE-3IND ‘These three hills are sown (with corn)’

Reduplication

Reduplication is an available but limited morphological process. Single vowel roots and some CV roots such as pá- (carry) are not reduplicated. About 70 roots never occur unreduplicated (niní-n (to ripen)). About a dozen reduplicate only the first syllable. In general, practically any root can stand either alone or reduplicated.

Three Systems:

  • k'waní-k'waní-ta-n (to throw up repeatedly) -- an aspectual system
  • meré-meré-hku-n (to be very brilliant) -- modal system
  • p'uní-p'uní-hku-n (to blow off all over (as when dusting)) -- locative system

Examples:

učú-učú-hku-ndi-n (to smell of fish, sex, or carrion) -- spatial suffix (oral and facial, involves smell) opó-opó-k-haśi (swollen (of the entire body)) -- the roots may be followed by one of six consonants and the stativizing suffix -hási ‘class, type of’, as in opó- ‘large, round’ kwená-hku-n (to lick upward (as of fire)) -- roots are followed by intransitivizing -hku wá-wá-kuri-n (to be missing one’s teeth, leaves, etc.) -- reflexive theme arí-arí-ta-n (to give advice) -- yield a transitive theme yará-yará-c-ka-pa-n (to go about urinating on oneself) -- lexical/spatial suffix value of ‘bottom’ šún-šún-á-pi-ti (very green) -- only first CV is reduplicated tíks-tíks-á-ni-n (to beat of fright) -- cavity/thorax spatial suffix